Absolute Pitch Acquisition as Memory System with Its' Musical Executive Function from Cognitive Neuropsychological Perspective

Authors

  • Irina G. Andreeva Laboratory of comparative physiology of sensory systems, I. M. Sechenov Institute of Evolutionary Physiology and Biochemistry, Russian Academy of Sciences, Toreza pr.44,194223 Saint-Petersburg.
  • Maria Dymnikowa Laboratory of Psychophysiology of Speech, I. P. Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Makarova nab. 6, 199034 Saint-Petersburg. Association of Musical Psychologists and Psychotherapists, Akademika Anohina st. 38/3, 119602, Moscow. Laboratory of Music-therapy, Institute of Psychiatry and Neurology, al. Sobieskiego 1/9, 02957 Warsaw.
  • Elena A. Ogorodnikova Laboratory of Psychophysiology of Speech, I. P. Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Makarova nab. 6, 199034 Saint-Petersburg.
  • Valentin I. Petrushin Association of Musical Psychologists and Psychotherapists, Akademika Anohina st. 38/3, 119602, Moscow. P. I. Tchaikovsky Moscow State Conservatory, Bolshaya Nikitskaya st. 13/6, 125009 Moscow.

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24203/ajhss.v7i5.5900

Keywords:

absolute pitch, executive function, long-term working memory, music memory training

Abstract

Absolute (perfect) pitch is defined as the ability to identify and categorize a musical pitch - tone frequency by its name in the musical equal temperament tuning system without an external reference tone.    The article presents absolute pitch process as long-term working semantic musical memory with musical executive function included in musical practical activity, with the description of similarities between music and language structure in music cognitive neuropsychology studies’ evidence, as a background for cognitive neuropsychological possibilities of its generally available development. The article describes methodological assumptions’ content of empirically developed aural behavioral psychological memory training method ‘absolute pitch ecological practical activity’ (APEPA) for childhood and youth age, based on working memory span, aural cognitive learning ability in music education and performance activity, in cognitive music neuropsychology field.

 

References

Abraham, O. (1901). Das absolute Tonbewusstsein. Psychologisch-musikalische studie. Sammelbände der internationalen musikgesellschaft, 3, p.1-86.

Adams, W. (2006). The mysteries of perfect pitch. http://www.psychologytoday.com/articles/200607/the-mysteries-perfect-pitch (access on March 31st 2019).

Amiez, C., Kostopoulos, P., Champod, A. S., Petrides, M. (2006). Local

morphology predicts functional organization of the dorsal premotor region in the human brain. Journal of neuroscience, March, 26 (10), p.2724-2731.

Amunts, K., Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Steinmetz, H., Schleicher, A., Dabringhaus, A., Zilles, K. (1997). Motor cortex and hand motor skills: Structural compliance in the human brain. Human brain mapping, 5 (3), p.206-215.

ANSI (1999). American National Psychoacoustic terminology. S3.20. ed. New York, American national standards institute. 67 p.

Anvari, S. H., Trainor, L. J., Woodside, J., Levy, B. A. (2002). Relations among musical skills, phonological processing, and early reading ability in prescholl children. Journal of experimental child psychology, October, 83 (2), p.111-130.

Arlinger, S., Lunner, T., Lyxell, B., Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2009). The emergence of cognitive hearing science. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 50 (5), p.371-384.

Auzou, P., Eustache, F., Etevenon, P., Platel, H., Rioux, P., Lambert, J., Lechevalier, B., Zarifian, E., Baron, J. C. (1995). Topographic EEG activations during timbre and pitch discrimination tasks using musical sounds. Neuropsychologia, January, 33 (1), p.25-37.

Ayotte, J., Hyde., K., Peretz, I. (2002). Congenital amusia: a group study of adults afflicted with a music-specific disorder. Brain: a journal of neurology, February, 125 (2), p.238-251.

Bachem, A. (1937). Various types of absolute pitch. Journal of the acoustical society of America, October, 9, p.146-151.

Bachem, A. (1954). Time factors in relative and absolute pitch determination. Journal of the acoustical society of America, September, 26 (5), p.751 - 753.

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. ed. Oxford clarendon press. 289 p.

Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, January, 255(5044), p.556-559.

Baddeley, A. D. (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current opinion in neurobiology, April, 8 (2), p.234-238.

Baddeley, A. D. (2002). The psychology of memory. In: A. D. Baddeley, B. A. Wilson, M. D. Kopelman. (eds.). Handbook of Memory Disorders. ed. Hove, Psychology press. p.3-15.

Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of learning and motivation, vol.8. p.47-89.

Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working Memory. In: G. A. Bower (ed.). Recent advances in learning and motivation. vol.8. ed. New York Academic Press, p.47-90.

Baharloo, S., Johnston, P. A., Service, S. K., Gitschier, J., Freimer, N. B. (1998). Absolute pitch: an approach for identification of genetic and nongenetic components. American journal of human genetics, February, 62 (2), p. 224-231.

Baharloo, S., Service, S. K., Risch, N., Gitschier, J., Freimer, N. B. (2000). Familial aggregation of absolute pitch. American journal of human genetics, 67 (3), p.755-758.

Bahr, N. (1998). Pitch discrimination skill: a cognitive perspective. Paper presented at the Annual conference of the Australian Association for Research in Education, Adelaide. 21p.

Bahr, N. (2005). Diversity of accuracy profiles for absolute pitch recognition. Psychology of music, 33 (1), p.58-93.

Bahr, N., Christensen, C. A., Bahr, M. (2005). Diversity of accuracy profiles for absolute pitch recognition. Psychology of music, 33 (1), p.58-93.

Balari, S., Lorenzo, G. (2009). Computational phenotypes: Where the theory of computation meets evo-devo. Biolinguistics, 3(1), p.2-60.

Barwick, J., Valentine, E., West, R., Wilding, J. (1989). Relations between reading and musical abilities. British journal of educational psychology, June, 59 (2), p.253-257.

Belin, P., Zatorre, R. J., Lafaille, P., Ahad, P., Pike, B. (2000). Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex. Nature, January, 403 (6767), p.309-312.

Bendor, D, Wang, X (2005). The neuronal representation of pitch in primate auditory cortex. Nature, August, 436 (7054), p.1161-1165.

Bergmann, H. C., Rijpkema, M., Fernandez, G., Kessels, R.P. (2012). Distinct neural correlates of associative working memory and long-term memory encoding in the medial temporal lobe. Neuroimage, November, 63 (2), p.989-997.

Bermudez, P., Zatorre, R. J. (2005). Conditional associative memory for musical stimuli in nonmusicians: implications for absolute pitch. The journal of neuroscience, August, 25 (34), p.7718-7723.

Bermudez, P., Zatorre, R. J. (2009). The absolute pitch mind continues to reveal itself. Journal of biology, August, 8, art. 75, 4 p.

Bermudez, P., Zatorre, R. J. (2009). A distribution of absolute pitch ability as revealed by computerized testing. Music perception, 27 (2), p.89-101.

Bever, T. G., Chiarello, R. J. (1974). Cerebral dominance in musicians and nonmusicians. Science, August, 185 (4150), p.537-539.

Bianki, W. L. (1985). Animal brain asymmetry. ed. Leningrad, Nauka. 296 p. [original title: Asimmetria mozga ziwotnyh.]

Bidelman, G. M., Gandour, J. T., Krishnan, A. (2011). Musicians and tone-language speakers share enhanced brainstem encoding but not perceptual benefits for musical pitch. Brain and cognition, October, 77 (1), p.1-10.

Binder, J. R., Rao, S. M., Hammeke, T. A., Yetkin, F. Z., Jesmanowicz, A., Bandettini, P. A., Wong, E. C., Estkowski, L. D., Goldstein, M. D., Haughton, V. M., Hyde, J. S. (1994). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human auditory cortex. Annals of neurology, June, 35 (6), p.662-672.

Bliss, T. V., Lomo, T. (1973). Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. Journal of physiology, July, 232 (2), p.331-356.

Boettiger, C. A., D'Esposito, M. (2005). Frontal networks for learning and executing arbitrary stimulus-response associations. Journal of neuroscience, March, 25 (10), p.2723-2732.

Brady, P. T. (1970). Fixed-scale mechanism of absolute pitch. Journal of the acoustical society of America, 48, p.883-887.

Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. ed. London, England, Academic press. 522 p.

Brown, M. W., Aggleton, J. P. (2001). Recognition memory: what are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nature reviews. Neuroscience, January, 2 (1), p.51-61.

Brown, W., Cammuso, K., Sachs, H., Winklosky, B., Mullane, J., Bernier, R., Svenson, S., Arin, D., Rosen-Sheidley, Folstein, S. (2003). Autism-related language, personality and cognition in people with absolute pitch: results of a preliminary study. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 33 (2), p.163-166.

Buonomano, D. V., Merzenich, M. M. (1998). Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. Annual review of neuroscience, 21, p.149-186.

Burns, E. M., Campbell, S. L. (1994). Frequency and frequency-ratio resolution by possessors of absolute and relative pitch: examples of categorical perception. Journal of the acoustical society of America, November, 96, p.2704 - 2719.

Caplan, J. B., Madsen, J. R., Raghavachari, S., Kahana, M. J. (2001). Distinct patterns of brain oscillations underlie two basic parameters of human maze learning. Journal of neurophysiology, July, 86 (1), p.368-380.

Chan, A. S., Ho, Y. C., Cheung, M. C. (1998). Music training improves verbal memory. Nature, November, 396 (6707), p.128.

Chase, W. G., Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In: G.H. Bower (ed.). The psychology of learning and motivation. vol.16. ed. New

York Academic press. p.1-58.

Chen, J., Dastjerdi, M., Foster, B. L., LaRocque, K. F., Rauschecker, A. M., Parvizi, J., Wagner, A. D. (2013). Human hippocampal increases in low-frequency power during associative prediction violations. Neuropsychologia, October, 51 (12), p.2344-2351.

Chin, C. S. (2003). The development of absolute pitch: a theory concerning the roles of musical training at an early developmental age and individual cognitive style. Psychology of music, 31, p.155-171.

Cooper-Kahn, J., Dietzel, L. (2019). What is executive functioning? http://www.ldonline.org/article/29122 (access on March 31st 2019).

Coulmas, F. (1989). The writing systems of the world. ed. Oxford, Basil Blackwell publishers ltd. 302 p.

Cowan, N. A. (1998). Visual and auditory working memory capacity. Trends in cognitive sciences, 2 (3), p.77.

Cowan, N. A. (1999). An embedded - processes model of working memory. In: A. Miyake, P. Shah (ed.). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Ed. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, p.62-101.

Cowan, N. A. (2001). The magical number 4 in short - term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and brain sciences, February, 24 (1). p.87-185.

Cowan, N. A. (2005). Working memory capacity. Essays in Cognitive Psychology. ed. Psychology Press, Taylor and Francis Group, New York, LLC. 246 p.

Cowan, N. A. (2008). What are the differences between long -term, short -term, and working memory? Progress in brain research, vol.169. p.323-338.

Cowan, N. A., Morey, C. C., AuBuchon, A. M., Zwilling, C. E., Gilchrist, A. L. (2010). Seven - year - olds allocate attention like adults unless working memory is overloaded. Developmental science, 13 (1), p.120-133.

Crespo-Garcia, M., Cantero, J. L., Atienza, M. (2012). Effects of semantic relatedness on age-related associative memory deficits: the role of theta oscillations. Neuroimage, 61 (4), p.1235-1248.

Crowne, D. P., Dawson, K. A., Richardson, C. M. (1989). Unilateral periarcuate and posterior parietal lesions impair conditional position discrimination learning in the monkey. Neuropsychologia, 27 (9), p.1119-1127.

Daniels, P. T., Bright, W. (1996). The world's writing systems. ed. Oxford University Press. 968 p.

Davies, J. B. (1978). The psychology of music. ed. Stanford university press. 240 p.

Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002). Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science, December, 298 (5600), p.2013-2015.

Deutsch, D. (1970). Tones and numbers: specificity of interference in immediate memory. Science, June, 168 (3939), p.1604-1605.

Deutsch, D. (1991). The tritone paradox: an influence of language on music perception. Music perception, July, 8 (4), p.335-347.

Deutsch, D. (2002). The puzzle of absolute pitch. Journal of American psychological society, 11 (6), p.200-204.

Deutsch, D. (2006). Absolute pitch, structural acoustics, ASA history, and more. Acoustics today, October, 2 (4), p.17.

Deutsch, D. (2013). Absolute pitch. In: D. deutsch (ed.). The psychology of music. ed. San Diego, CA, Elsevier, p.141-182.

Deutsch, D. (2013). Psychology of music. ed. San Diego, CA, Elsevier. 786 p.

Deutsch, D., Dooley, K. (2013). Absolute pitch is associated with a large auditory digit span: a clue to its genesis. Journal of the acoustical society of America, April, 133 (4), p.1859-1861.

Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Dolson, M. (2004). Absolute pitch, speech, and tonal language: some experiments and a proposed framework. Music perception, 21 (3), p.339-356.

Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., Xu, H. (2006). Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory studies: prevalence differences, and evidences for a speech-related critical period. Journal of the acoustical society of America, February,119 (2), p.719-722.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, vol.64, p.135-168.

Dobbins, I. G., Rice, H. J., Wagner, A. D., Schacter, D. L. (2003). Memory orientation and success: separable neurocognitive components underlying episodic recognition. Neuropsychologia, 41 (3), p.318-333.

Dohn, A., Garza-Villarreal, E. A., Riisgaard, R. L., Wallentin, M., Vuust, P. (2014). Musical activity tunes up absolute pitch ability. Music Perception, 31 (4), p.359-371.

Drayna, D., Manichaikul, A., de Lange, M., Snieder, H., Spector, T. (2001). Genetic correlates of musical pitch recognition in humans. Science, March, 291 (5510), p.1969-1972.

Dymnikowa, M. (2019). Analysis of sight-reading musical psychological process as an executive function. [original title: Analiz muzykalnogo psihologiceskogo processa ctenia s lista kak ispolnitelnoj funkcii.] The scientific method, 27 (1), p.3-9. http://scientific-met.com/wp-content/uploads/2019/04/SMT_27.pdf (access on March 31st 2019).

Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological review, 66, p.183-201.

Eguchi, K. (1991). Zettai onkan program. Ed. Tokyo, Japan, Zen-on Gakufu Shuppan.

Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, October, 1 (1), p.41-50.

Eimas, P. D. (1975). Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: discrimination of the /r-l/ distinction by young infants. Perception and psychophysics, 18 (5), p.341-347.

Eimas, P. D., Siquel, E. R., Juszczyk, P., Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, January, 171 (3968), p.303-306.

Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, October, 270 (5234), p.305-307.

Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., Engel, S. A. (2000). Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nature neuroscience, November, 3 (11), p.1149-1152.

Elmer, S., Sollberger, S., Meyer, M., Jancke, L. (2013). An empirical reevaluation of absolute pitch: behavioral and electrophysiological measurements. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, October, 25 (10), p.1736-1753.

Ergorul, C., Eichenbaum, H. (2004). The hippocampus and memory for "what," "where," and "when". Learning and memory, July, 11 (4), p.397-405.

Ericsson, K. A., Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, April, 102 (2), p.211-245.

Eysenck, M. W., Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and emotion, 6, p.409-434.

Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: is the melody the message? Child development, December, 60 (6), p.1497-1510.

Frankland, P. W., Bontempi, B. (2005). The organization of recent and remote memories. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, February, 6 (2), p.119-130.

Friederici, A. D. (ed.). (1998). Language comprehension: a biological perspective. ed. Springer - Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. 314 p.

Friederici, A. D., Meyer, M., von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Auditory language comprehension: an event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information. Brain and language, December, 75 (3), P.289-300.

Fuster, J. M. (2002). Physiology of executive function: the perception-action cycle. In: D. T. Stuss, R. T. Knight (eds.). Principles of frontal lobe function. ed. New York, Oxford university press, p.96-109.

Galinska, E. (1989). La musicotherapie cognitive: le portrait musical du patient. La Revue de Musicothérapie, January, 1, p.33-63.

Galinska, E. (2008). Music as a carrier of human traits: the musical identity test (MIT). Przeglad psychologiczny, 51 (4), p.423-441.

Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: the theory of multiple intelligences. ed. New York, Basic books. 440 p.

Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. ed. New York, Basic Books. 423 p.

Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: multiple intelligences for the 21st century. ed. New York, Basic Books. 292 p.

Gardner, H., Hatch, T. (1989). Multiple intelligences go to school: Educational implications of the theory of multiple intelligences. Educational researcher, 18 (8), p.4-10.

Gardner, H., Kornhaber, M. L., Wake, W. K. (1996). Intelligence: multiple perspectives. ed. Harcourt Brace College Publishers. 351 p.

Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., Mangun, G. R. (2013). Cognitive neuroscience: the biology of mind. ed. New York, London, W.W. Norton and company. 752p.

Glass, G. V., Willson, V. L., Gottman, J. M. (2008). Design and analysis of time-series experiments. ed. Information Age Publishing. 264 p.

Gobet, F. (1998). Expert memory: a comparison of four theories. Cognition, 66 (2), p.115-152.

Goldberg, E., Costa, L. D. (1981). Hemisphere differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems. Brain and language, September, 14 (1), p.144-173.

Gordon, E. E. (1965). Musical aptitude profile. ed. Boston, MA, Houghton Mifflin Company (1965), GIA Music (2005), 162 p.

Gordon, E. E. (1967). The Musical Aptitude Profile. Music educators journal, 53 (6), p.52-54.

Gordon, E. E. (1988). Learning sequences in music: skill, content, and patterns. ed. Chicago, G. I. A. Publications. 354 p.

Gordon, E. E. (1997). Learning sequences in music: skill, content and patterns. A music learning theory. ed. Chicago, G. I. A. Publications. 397 p.

Gordon, E. E. (2006). Nature, source, measurement and evaluation of musical aptitudes. Polish psychological forum, 11 (2), p.227-237.

Gordon, E. E. (2007). Learning sequences in music: a contemporary learning theory. ed. Chicago, G. I. A. Publications. 448 p.

Gregersen, P. K., Kowalsky, E., Kohn, N., Marvin, E. W. (1999). Absolute pitch: prevalence, ethnic variation, and estimation of the genetic component. American journal of medical genetics, September, 65 (3), p.911-913.

Gregersen, P. K., Kowalsky, E., Kohn, N., Marvin, E. W. (2000). Early childhood music education and predisposition to absolute pitch: teasing apart genes and environment. American journal of medical genetics, January, 98 (3), p.280-282.

Griffiths, T. D., Warren, J. D. (2002). The planum temporale as a computational hub. Trends in neurosciences, July, 25 (7), p.348-353.

Griusser, P., Zelke, A., Cinda, T. (1995). Functional asymmetry of the brain and its importance for art, aesthetic perception and artistic creation. In: I. Rebcler, B. Hercberger, D. Epstain. Beauty and the brain. Biological aspects of aesthetics. ed. Moscow, Mir. Chapter 11, p.265-299. [original title: Funkcionalnaja asimmetria mozga i eje znacenie dla iskusstwa, esteticeskogo wospritaia i hudozestwennogo tworcestwa. In: Krasota i mozg. Biologiceskie aspekty estetiki.]

Guida, A., Gobet, F., Tardieu, H., Nicolas, S. (2012). How chunks, long-term working memory and templates offer a cognitive explanation for neuroimaging data on expertise acquisition: a two-stage framework. Brain and cognition, August, 79 (3), p.221-244.

Halpern, A. R. (1989). Memory for absolute pitch of familiar songs. Memory and cognition, September, 17 (5), p.572-581.

Halsband, U., Freund, H. J. (1990). Premotor cortex and conditional motor learning in man. Brain, February, 113 (1), p.207-222.

Halsband, U., Passingham, R. E. (1985). Premotor cortex and the conditions for movement in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Behavioural Brain Research, December, 18 (3), p.269-277.

Hantz, E. C., Crummer, G. C., Wayman, J. W., Walton, J. P., Frisina, R. D. (1992). Effects of musical training and absolute pitch on the neural processing of melodic intervals: a P3 event-related potential study. Music perception, Fall, 10 (1), p.25-42.

Hantz, E. C., Kreilick, K. G., Braveman, A. L., Swartz, K. P. (1995). Effects of musical training and absolute pitch on a pitch memory task an event-related-potential study. Psychomusicology: a journal of research in music cognition, 14 (1-2), p.53-76.

Hantz, E. C., Kreilick, K. G., Kananen, W., Swartz, K. P. (1997). Neural responses to melodic and harmonic closure: an event-related-potential study. Music perception, 15 (1), p.69-98.

Harris, C. A. (1926). Can I develop absolute pitch? The etude, 44, p.721-722.

Hartmann, D. P., Gottman, J. M., Jones, R. R., Gardner, W., Kazdin, A. E., Vaught, R. (1980). Interrupted time-series analysis and its application to behavioral data. Journal of applied behavior analysis, 13, p.543-559.

Hauser, M. D., McDermott, J. (2003). The evolution of the music faculty: a comparative perspective. Nature neuroscience, July, 6 (7), p.663-668.

Hay, A. (2009). Stability and accuracy of long-term memory for musical pitch. BA thesis. ed., Edith Cowan University, 31 p. https://ro.ecu.edu.au/theses_hons/1207/ (access on March 31st 2019).

Headley, D. B., Pare, D. (2013). In sync: gamma oscillations and emotional memory. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, November, vol.7, art.170, 12 p.

Heald, Sh. L. M., Van Hedger, S. C., Nusbaum, H. C. (2017). Understanding sound: auditory skill acquisition. Psychology of learning and motivation, vol.67, p.53-93.

Heath, S., McMahon, K., Nickels, L., Angwin, A., MacDonald, A., van Hees, S., Johnson, K., Copland, D. (2012). The neural correlates of picture naming facilitated by auditory repetition. BMC Neuroscience, vol.13, art.21, 13 p.

Heiervang, E., Hugdahl, K., Steinmetz, H., Smievoll, I. A., Stevenson, J., Lund, A., Ersland, L., Lundervold, A.,. (2000). Planum temporale, planum parietale, and dichotic listening in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 38 (13), p.1704-1713.

Heilman, K. M., Van Den Abell, T. (1980). Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanisms underlying hemispheres asymmetries of innatention (neglect). Neurology, March, 30 (3), p.327-330.

Helmholtz, H. F., Ellis, A. J. (1885). On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music. 2nd ed. London, New York, Longmans, Green and co. 576 p. Reprint: 2012. Series Cambridge library collection - music. 3rd ed. Cambridge university press.

Hirata, Y., Kuriki, S., Pantev, C. (1999). Musicians with absolute pitch show distinct neural activities in the auditory cortex. Neuroreport, April, 10 (5), p.999-1002.

Hirose, H., Kubota, M., Kimura, I., Ohsawa, M., Yumoto, M., Sakakihara, Y. (2002). People with absolute pitch process tones with producing P300. Neuroscience letters, September, 330 (3), p.247-250.

Hirose, H., Kubota, M., Kimura, I., Yumoto, M., Sakakihara, Y. (2004).

N100m in adults possessing absolute pitch. Neuroreport, June, 15 (9), p.1383-1386.

Hirshhorn, M., Grady, C., Rosenbaum, R. S., Winocur, G., Moscovitch, M. (2012). Brain regions involved in the retrieval of spatial and episodic details associated with a familiar environment: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, November, 50 (13), p.3094-3106.

Ho, Y. C., Cheung, M. C., Chan, A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology, July, 17 (3), p.439-450.

Huang, Z., Zhang, J. X., Yang, Z., Dong, G., Wu, J., Chan, A. S., Weng, X. (2010). Verbal memory retrieval engages visual cortex in musicians. Neuroscience, June, 168 (1), p.179-189.

Itoh, K., Suwazono, S., Arao, H., Miyazaki, K., Nakada, T. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of absolute pitch and relative pitch. Cerebral cortex, June, 15 (6), p.760-769.

Ivry, R. B., Robertson, L. C. (1998). The two sides of perception. ed. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. 315 p.

Jackendoff, R., Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and what's special about it? Cognition, May, 100 (1), p.33-72.

Jamison, H. L., Watkins, K. E., Bishop, D. V., Matthews, P. M. (2006). Hemispheric specialization for processing auditory nonspeech stimuli. Cerebral cortex, September, 16 (9), p.1266-1275.

Jancke, L.,Wustenberg, T.,Scheich, H., Heinze, H. J. (2002).Phonetic perception and the temporal cortex. Neuroimage, 15, p.733-746.

Jefferies, L. N., Smilek, D., Eich, E., Enns, J. T. (2008). Emotional valence and arousal interact in attentional control. Psychological science, 19, p.290-295.

Johnsrude, I. S., Penhune, V. B., Zatorre, R. J. (2000). Functional specificity in the right human auditory cortex for perceiving pitch direction. Brain: a journal of neurology, January, 123 (1), p.155-163.

Jones, S. J., Longe, O., Vaz Pato, M. (1998). Auditory evoked potentials to abrupt pitch and timbre change of complex tones: electrophysiological evidence of ‘streaming’? Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology, March, 108 (2), p.131-142.

Jones, R. R., Vaught, R. S., Weinrott, M. (1977). Time-series analysis in operant research. Journal of applied behavior analysis, 10, p.151-166.

Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, October, 274 (5284), p.114-116.

Justus, K. (1988). Musical pitch identification by absolute pitch possessors. Perception and psychophysics, December, 44 (6), p.501-512.

Justus, T. C., Bharucha, J. J. (2002). Music perception and cognition. In: H. Pashler, S. Yantis (eds.). Steven's handbook of experimental psychology: sensation and perception. ed. Hoboken, NJ, US, John Wiley and Sons Inc.p.453-492.

Justus, T., Hutsler, J. J. (2005). Fundamental issues in the evolutionary psychology of music: assessing innateness and domain-specificity. Music perception, 23 (1), p.1-27.

Juszczyk, P. W., Krumhansl, C. L. (1993). Pitch and rhythmic patterns affecting infants’ sensitivity to musical phrase structure. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, June, 19 (3),

p.627-640.

Kaas, J. H., Hackett, T. A., Tramo, M. J. (1999). Auditory processing in primate cerebral cortex. Current opinion in neurobiology, April, 9 (2), p.164-170.

Kahana, M. J., Sekuler, R., Caplan, J. B., Kirschen, M., Madsen, J. R. (1999). Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual maze navigation. Nature, June, 399 (6738), p.781-784.

Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle. Psychological bulletin, 131 (1), p.66-71.

Keenan, J. P., Thangaraj, V., Halpern, A. R., Schlaug, G. (2001). Absolute pitch and planum temporale. Neuroimage, December, 14 (6), p.1402-1408.

Kelahan, A. M., Doetsch, G. S. (1984). Time-dependent changes in the functional organization of somatosensory cerebral cortex following digit amputation in adult raccoons. Somatosensory research, 2 (1), p.49-81.

Klein, M., Coles, M. G., Donchin, E. (1984). People with absolute pitch process tones without producing a P300. Science, March, 223 (4642), p.1306-1309.

Koelsch, S. (2005). Neural substrates of processing syntax and semantics in music. Current opinion in neurobiology, April, 15 (2), p. 207-212.

Koelsch, S., Siebel, W. A. (2005). Towards a neural basis of music perception. Trends in cognitive sciences, December, 9 (12), p.578-584.

Korsakowa, N. K., Moskowicute, L. I., Simernickaja, E. G. (1979). About functional interaction of the brain hemispheres in memory processes. In: Memory and trace processes. Abstracts of the 4 all-union conference. ed. Pusino, p.163-164. [original title: O funkcionalnom wzaimodeistwii polusharij mozga w procesah pamiati. In: Pamiat’ i sledowye processy.Tezisy dokladow IV wsesojuznoj konferencii.]

Kuhl, P. K. (1986). Theoretical contributions of tests on animals to the special-mechanisms debate in speech. Experimental biology, 45 (3), p.233-265.

Kuhl, P. K. (1991). Perception, cognition and the ontogenetic and phylogenetic emergence of human speech. In: S. E. Brauth, W. S. Hall, R. J. Dooling (eds.). Plasticity of development, ed. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. p.73-106.

Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Cracking the speech code: how infants learn language. Acoustical science and technology, 28 (2), p.71-83.

Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Nelson, T. (2007). Developmental phonetic perception: native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical transactions of the Royal society B. Biol. Sci.,363 (1493), p.979-1000.

Kuhnis, J., Elmer, S., Meyer, M., Jancke, L. (2013). The encoding of vowels and temporal speech cues in the auditory cortex of professional musicians: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia, July, 51 (8), p.1608-1618.

Kurata, K., Tsuji, T., Naraki, S., Seino, M., Abe, Y. (2000). Activation of the dorsal premotor cortex and pre-supplementary motor area of humans during an auditory conditional motor task. Journal of neurophysiology, September, 84 (3), p.1667-1672.

Langers, D. R. M., Krumbholz, K., Bowtell, R. W., Hall, D. A. (2014). Neuroimaging paradigms for tonotopic mapping (I): The influence of sound stimulus type. Neuroimage, October, 100, p.650-662.

Larsen, J. P., Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., Odegaard, H. (1990). MRI evaluation of the size and asymmetry of the planum temporale in adolescents with developmental dyslexia. Brain and language, August, 39 (2), p.289-301.

Lepage, M., Brodeur, M., Bourgouin, P. (2003). Prefrontal cortex contribution to associative recognition memory in humans: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience letters, July, 346 (1-2), p.73-76.

Leushina, L. I., Newskaja, A. A., Pawlowskaja, M. B. (1982). Asymmetry of the cerebral hemispheres in terms of identifying visual images. In: G. W. Gershuni (ed.). Sensory systems. Vision. ed. Leningrad, Nauka. p.76-92. [original title: Asimmetria polusarij golownogo mozga s tocki zrenija opoznanija zritelnyh obrazow. In: Sensornye sistemy. Zrenie.]

Levitin, D. J. (1994). Absolute memory for musical pitch: evidence from the production of learned melodies. Perception and psychophysics, 56 (4), p.414-423.

Levitin, D. J. (1996). Mechanisms of memory for musical attributes. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon, U.S.A. Dissertation Abstracts International, 57(07B), 4755. (University Microfilms No. AAG9638097).

Levitin, D. J. (1999). Absolute pitch: self-reference and human memory. International journal of computing and anticipatory systems, 4, p.255-266.

Levitin, D. J. (2004). Absolute pitch: self-reference and memory. (L'oreille absolue: Autoréférencement et mémoire) L’Annee psychologique, 104 (1), p.103-120.

Levitin, D. J., Menon, V. (2003). Musical structure is processed in "language" areas of the brain: a possible role for Brodmann area 47 in temporal coherence. Neuroimage, December, 20 (4), p.2142-2152.

Levitin, D. J., Rogers, S. E. (2005). Absolute pitch: perception, coding, and controversies. Trends in cognitive sciences, January, 9 (1), p.26-33.

Levitin, D. J., Zatorre, R. J. (2003). On the nature of early music training and absolute pitch: a reply to Brown, Sachs, Cammuso, and Folstein. Music perception, Fall, 21 (1), p.105-110.

Lewandowsky, S., Yang, L. X., Newell, B. R., Kalish, M. L. (2012). Working memory does not dissociate between different perceptual categorization tasks. Journal of experimental psychology: learning, memory, and cognition, 38 (4), p.881-904.

Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed. New York, Oxford university press. 1016 p.

Liegeois-Chauvel, C., Peretz, I., Babai, M., Laguitton, V., Chauvel, P. (1998). Contribution of different cortical areas in the temporal lobes to music processing. Brain, October, 121 (10), p.1853-1867.

Loring, D. W. (1999). Dictionary of neuropsychology. ed. New York, Oxford university press. 193 p.

Loui, P., Li, H. C., Schlaug, G. (2011). White matter integrity in right hemisphere predicts pitch-related grammar learning. Neuroimage, March, 55 (2), p.500-507.

Makovski, T. (2019). Preparing for distraction: Attention is enhanced prior to the presentation of distractors. Journal of experimental psychology: general, 148 (2), p.221-236.

Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., McInerney, S., Sorensen, A. G., Wang, R., Caviness, V. S., Pandya, D. N. (2005). Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cerebral cortex, June, 15 (6), p.854-869.

Massaro, D. W (1972). Perceptual images, processing time, and perceptual units in auditory perception. Psychological review, 79 (2), p.124-145.

Matsuda, A., Hara, K., Watanabe, S., Matsuura, M., Ohta, K., Matsushima, E. (2013). Pre-attentive auditory processing of non-scale pitch in absolute pitch possessors. Neuroscience letters, August, 548, p.155-158.

Maye, J., Werker, J. F., Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, January, 82 (3), B101-B111.

Meyer, M., Elmer, S., Ringli, M., Oechslin, M. S., Baumann, S., Jancke, L. (2011). Long-term exposure to music enhances the sensitivity of the auditory system in children. European journal of neuroscience, September, 34 (5), p. 755-765.

Mazoyer, B. M., Tzourio, N.Frak, V., Syrota, A., Murayama, N., Levrier, O., Salamon, G., Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Mehler, J. (1993). The cortical representation of speech. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, Fall, 5 (4), p.467-479.

Mazzucchi, A., Marchini, C., Budai, R., Parma, M. (1982). A case of receptive amusia with prominent timbre perception defect. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, July, 45 (7), p.644-647.

McCain, L. J., McCleary, R. (1979). The statistical analysis of the simple interrupted time-series quasi-experiment. In: T. D. Cook., D. T. Campbell, (eds.). Quasi-experimentation: design and analysis issues for field settings. Chicago, ed. Rand-Mc-Nally, p.233-293.

McLachlan, N., and Sarah, W. (2010). The central role of recognition in auditory perception: a neurobiological model. Psychological review, January, 117 (1), p.175-196.

Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Nazzi, T., Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (1996). Coping with linguistic diversity: the infant's viewpoint. In: J. L. Morgan, K. Demuth (eds.). Signal to syntax: bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. ed. Hillsdale, NJ, US, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. p.101-116.

Meltzer, J. A., Constable, R. T. (2005). Activation of human hippocampal formation reflects success in both encoding and cued recall of paired associates. Neuroimage, January, 24 (2), p.384-397.

Merzenich, M. M., Nelson, R. J., Stryker, M. P., Cynader, M. S., Schoppmann, A., Zook, J. M. (1984). Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. Journal of comparative neurology, April, 224 (4), p.591-605.

Meyer, M., Baumann, S., Jancke, L. (2006). Electrical brain imaging reveals spatio-temporal dynamics of timbre perception in humans. Neuroimage, October, 32 (4), p.1510-1523.

Meyer, M., Friederici, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Neurocognition of auditory sentence comprehension: event related fMRI reveals sensitivity to syntactic violations and task demands. Brain research, January, 9 (1), p.19-33.

Milner, B. (1962). Laterality effects in audition. In: V. B. Mountcastle (ed.). Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance. ed. Baltimore, Md. Johns Hopkins university press. p.177-195.

Miyazaki, K. (1989). Absolute pitch identification: Effects of timbre and pitch region. Music Perception, 7 (1), p.1-14.

Miyazaki, K. (2000). Interaction in musical-pitch naming and syllable naming: an experiment on a Stroop-like effect in hearing. In: T. Nakada (ed.). Integrated human brain science: theory, method, application (music). ed. Amsterdam, Elsevier. p.415-423.

Miyazaki, K., Ogawa, Y. (2006). Learning absolute pitch by children: a cross-sectional study. Music perception, 24 (1), p.63-78.

Morrison, A. B., Chein, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic bulletin and review, 18 (1), p.46-60.

Mummery, C. J., Ashburner, J., Scott, S. K., Wise, R. J. (1999). Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in six normal and two aphasic subjects. Journal of the acoustical society of America, July, 106 (1), p.449-457.

Munte, T. F., Altenmuller, E., Jancke, L. (2002). The musician's brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, June, 3 (6), p.473-478.

Munte, T. F., Nager, W., Beiss, T., Schroeder, C., Altenmuller, E. (2003). Specialization of the specialized: electrophysiological investigations in professional musicians. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, November, 999, p.131-139.

Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., Mehler, J. 1998. Language discrimination in newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rythm. Journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, June, 24 (3), p.756-766.

Norman, D. A., Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. In: R. Davidson, R. Schwartz, D. Shapiro. (eds.). Consciousness and self-regulation: advances in research and theory, volume IV. ed. New York, Plenum press. p.1-18.

Nusbaum, H, C., Morin, T. M. (1992). Paying attention to differences among talkers. In: Y. Tohkura, Y. Sagisaka, E. Vatikiotis-Bateson (eds.). Speech perception, production and linguistic structure. ed. Tokyo, OHM publishing company, p.113-134.

Oakes, W. E (1951). An alternative interpretation of "absolute pitch." Transactions of the Kansas academy of sciences, 54, p.396-406.

Oechslin, M. S., Meyer, M., Jancke, L. (2010). Absolute pitch - functional evidence of speech relevant auditory acuity. Cerebral cortex, February, 20 (2), p.447-455.

Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Asada, T., Aruga, M., Hirakata, M., Nishikawa, M., Katoh, A., Imabayashi, E. (2001). Functional anatomy of musical perception in musicians. Cerebral cortex, August, 11 (8), p.754-760.

Olivers, C. N., Nieuwenhuis, S. (2005). The beneficial effect of concurrent task-irrelevant mental activity on temporal attention. Psychological science, 16, p.265-269.

Oskina, S. E., Parnes, D. G. (2005). Musical hearing: theory and method of development and improvement. ed. Moscow, AST. 78 p. [original title: Muzykalnyj sluh: teoria i metodika razwitia i sowershenstwowania.]

Overy, K. (2003). Dyslexia and music. From timing deficits to musical intervention. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, November, 999, p.497-505.

Owen, A. M., Milner, B., Petrides, M., Evans, A. C. (1996). Memory for object features versus memory for object location: a positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, August, 93 (17), p.9212-9217.

Pantev, C., Engelien, A., Candia, V., Elbert, T. (2001). Representational cortex in musicians.Plastic alterations in response to musical practice. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, June, 930, p.300-314.

Pantev, C., Roberts, L. E., Schulz, M., Engelien, A., Ross, B. (2001). Timbre-specific enhancement of auditory cortical representations in musicians. Neuroreport, January, 12 (1), p.169-174.

Papousek, H. (1996). Musicality in infancy research: biological and cultural origins of early musicality. In: I. Deliege., J. A. Sloboda. (eds.) Musical beginnings. ed. Oxford University Press. p.37-55.

Parncutt, R., Levitin, D. J. (2001). Absolute pitch. In: S. Sadie (ed.). New Grove dictionary of music and musicians. ed. New York, St. Martin’s press. p.37-39.

Patel, A. D. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature neuroscience, July, 6 (7), p. 674-681.

Patel, A. D. 2010. Music, language, and the brain. ed. New York, Oxford University Press. 526 p.

Pauws, S. (2003). Effects of song familiarity, singing training, and recent song exposure on the singing of melodies. 8 p. https://jscholarship.library.jhu.edu/handle/1774.2/11 (access on March 31st 2019).

Pechmann, T., Mohr, G. (1992). Interference in memory for tonal pitch: implications for a working-memory model. Memory and cognition, 20 (3), p.314-320.

Penfield, W., Milner, B. (1958). Memory deficit produced by bilateral lesions in the hippocampal zone. A.M.A.Archives of neurology and psychiatry, May, 79 (5). p.475-497.

Perani, D., Saccuman, M. C., Scifo, P., Spada, D., Andreolli, G., Rovelli, R., Baldoli, C., Koelsch, S. (2010). Functional specializations for music processing in the human newborn brain. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, March, 107 (10), p.4758-4763.

Peretz, I., Zatorre, R, J. (2005). Brain organization for music processing. Annual review of psychology, 56, p.89-114.

Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., Kenzer, A., Bauer, A. (2012). Interactions between cognition and emotion during response inhibition. Emotion, 12, p.192-197.

Petrides, M. (1985). Deficits in non-spatial conditional associative learning after periarcuate lesions in the monkey. Behavioural brain research, August, 16 (2-3), p.95-101.

Petrides, M. (1990). Nonspatial conditional learning impaired in patients with unilateral frontal but not unilateral temporal lobe excisions. Neuropsychologia, 28 (2), p.137-149.

Petrides, M. (1985). Deficits on conditional associative-learning tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 23 (5), p.601-614.

Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E. (1993). Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, February, 90 (3), p.873-877.

Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Meyer, E., Evans, A. C. (1993). Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, February, 90 (3), p.878-882.

Podbros, L. Z., Stamm, J. S., Denaro, F. J. (1980). Associative function of the arcuate segment of the monkey's prefrontal cortex. Physiology and behavior, January 24 (1), p.103-109.

Poeppel, D. (1996). A critical review of PET studies of phonological processing. Brain and language, December, 55 (3), p.317-385.

Poppel, E. (1994). Temporal mechanisms in perception. International review of neurobiology, 37, p.185-207.

Poppel, E. (1997). A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends in cognitive sciences, May, 1 (2), p.56-61.

Poppel, E. (2004). Lost in time: a historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3-second-window. Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis, 64 (3), p.295-301.

Profita, J., Bidder, T. G., Optiz, J. M., Reynolds, J. F. (1988). Perfect pitch. American journal of medical genetics, April, 29 (4), p.763-771.

Rabbitt, P. (1997). Methodology of frontal and executive function. ed.

Hove, UK, Psychology press. 264 p.

Ragert, P., Schmidt, A., Altenmuller, E., Dinse, H. R. (2004). Superior

tactile performance and learning in professional pianists: evidence for meta-plasticity in musicians. European journal of neuroscience, January, 19 (2), p.473-478.

Rakowski, A. (1993). Categorical perception in absolute pitch. Archives of Acoustics, 18 (4), p. 515-523.

Rakowski, A., Morawska-Bungeler, M. (1987). In search for the criteria of absolute pitch. Archives of acoustics, 12, p.75-87.

Rakowski, A., Miyazaki, K. (2007). Absolute pitch: common traits in music and language. Archives of acoustics, 32 (1), p.5-16.

Rama, P., Relander-Syrjanen, K., Carlson, S., Salonen, O., Kujala, T. (2012). Attention and semantic processing during speech: An fMRI study. Brain and language. August, 122 (2), p.114-119.

Ramus, F., Nespor, M., Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, December, 73 (3), p.265-292.

Revesz, G. (1913). Zur grundlagen der tonpsychologie. Ed. Germany, Leipzig Veit.

Robertson, D., Irvine, D. R. (1989). Plasticity of frequency organization in auditory cortex of guinea pigs with partial unilateral deafness. Journal of comparative neurology, April, 282 (3), p.456-471.

Rogenmoser, L., Elmer, S., Jancke, L. (2014). Absolute pitch: evidence for early cognitive facilitation during passive listening as revealed by reduced P3a amplitudes. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, March, 27 (3), p.623-637.

Rotenberg, V. S., Arshavsky, V. V. (1995): The "entropy" of right hemisphere activity and the restorative capacity of image thinking. Journal of mental imagery, 19, p.151-160.

Ronnberg, J., Lunner, Th., Zekveld, A., Sorqvist, P., Danielsson, H., Lyxell, B., Dahlstrom, O., Signoret, C., Stenfelt, S., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Rudner, M. (2013). The ease of language understanding (ELU) model: theoretical, empirical, and clinical advances. Frontiers in systems neuroscience, July, vol.7, art.31, 17 p.

Ross, D., Olson, I., Marks, L., Gore, J. (2004). A nonmusical paradigm for identifying absolute pitch possessors. Journal of the acoustical society of America, October, 116 (3), p.1793-1799.

Ross, D. A., Olson, I. R., Gore, J. C. (2006). Absolute pitch does not depend on early musical training. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, November, 999, p.522-526.

Saffran, J. R, Aslin, R. N., Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month old infants. Science, December, 274 (5294), p.1926-1928.

Sakakibara. A. (2014). A longitudinal study of the process of acquiring absolute pitch: a practical report of training with the ‘chord identification method’. Psychology of music, 42 (1), p.86-111.

Sakurai, Y. (2002). Coding of auditory temporal and pitch information by hippocampal individual cells and cell assemblies in the rat. Neuroscience, 115 (4), p.1153-1163.

Salame, P., Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Effects of background music on phonological short-term memory. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology: a human experimental psychology, 41A, p. 107-122.

Samson, S. (2003). Neuropsychological studies of musical timbre. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, November, 999 (1), p.144-151.

Samson, S., Zatorre, R. J. (1988). Melodic and harmonic discrimination following unilateral cerebral excision. Brain and cognition, June, 7 (3), p.348-360.

Samson, S., Zatorre, R. J. (1994). Contribution of the right temporal lobe to musical timbre discrimination. Neuropsychologia, February, 32 (2), p.231-240.

Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Schabus, M., Doppelmayr, M. (2005). Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. International journal of psychophysiology, August, 57 (2), p.97-103.

Schellenberg, E. G., Trehub, S. E. (2003). Good pitch memory is widespread. Psychological science, May. 14 (3), p.262-266.

Schendel, Z. A, Palmer, C. (2007). Suppression effects on musical and verbal memory. Memory and cognition, 35 (4), p.640-650.

Schlaug, G. (2001). The brain of musicians. A model for functional and structural adaptation. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, June, 930, p.281-299.

Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y., Steinmetz, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science, 267 (5198), p.699-701.

Schlosser, M. J., Aoyagi, N., Fulbright, R. K., Gore, J. C., McCarthy, G. (1998). Functional MRI studies of auditory comprehension. Human brain mapping, 6 (1), p.1-13.

Schonwiesner, M., Rubsamen, R., von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). Hemispheric asymmetry for spectral and temporal processing in the human antero-lateral auditory belt cortex. European journal of neuroscience, September, 22 (6), p.1521-1528.

Schreiner, C. E., Read, H. L., Sutter, M. L. (2000). Modular organization of frequency integration in primary auditory cortex. Annual review of neuroscience, 23, p.501-529.

Schulze, K., Gaab, N., Schlaug, G. (2009). Perceiving pitch absolutely: comparing absolute and relative pitch possessors in a pitch memory task. BMC neuroscience, August,10, art. 106, 13 p.

Seachore, C. E. (1919). The psychology of musical talent. ed. Boston, Silver Burdett Company. 310 p.

Semenowic, A. W. (2018). Introduction to neuropsychology of childhood. 6th ed. Moscow, Genesis. 319 p. [original title: Wwedenie w neiropsihologiu detskogo wozrasta.]

Sergeant, D. (1969). Experimental investigation of absolute pitch. Journal of research in music education, 17, p.135-143.

Sergeant, D., Roche, S. (1973). Perceptual shifts in the auditory information processing of young children. Psychology of music, 1 (2), p.39-48.

Shallice, T. Burgess, P. (1996). The domain of supervisory processes and the temporal organization of behavior. Philosophical transactions of the Royal society of London, 351, p.1405-1412.

Shallice, Т. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical transactions of the Royal society of London, 298, p.199-209.

Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Constable, R. T., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R. K., Bronen, R. A., Fletcher, J. M., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Gore, J. C. (1995). Sex differences in the functional organization of the brain for language. Nature, February, 373 (6515). p.607-609.

Siegel, J. A. (1974). Sensory and verbal coding strategies in subjects with absolute pitch. Journal of experimental psychology, July, 103 (1), p.37-44.

Siegel, J. A., Siegel, W. (1977). Absolute identification of notes and intervals by musicians. Perception and Psychophysics, 21 (2), p.143-152.

Sinai, A., Pratt, H. (2003). High-resolution time course of hemispheric dominance revealed by low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. Clinical neurophysiology, July, 114 (7), p.1181-1188.

Stamm, J. S. (1973). Functional dissociation between the inferior and arcuate segments of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the monkey. Neuropsychologia, May, 11 (2), p.181-190.

Stanley, J. M., Hughes, J. E. (1997). Evaluation of an early intervention music curriculum for enhancing prereading / writing skills. Music therapy perspectives, 15, p.79-85.

Stumpf, C. (1882, 1890). Tonpsychology, vol.1 (1882), vol.2 (1890), ed. Hirzel, Leipzig.

Stuss, D. T., Benson, D. F. (1986). The frontal lobes. ed. New York, Raven press. 303 p.

Stuss, D. T., Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal lobe function. ed. New York, Oxford university press. 800 p.

Sussman, T. J., Heller, W., Miller, G. A., Mohanty, A. (2013). Emotional distractors can enhance attention. Psychological science, 24 (11), p.2322-2328.

Suzuki, W. A., Clayton, N. S. (2000). The hippocampus and memory: a comparative and ethological perspective. Current opinion in neurobiology, December, 10 (6), p.768-773.

Szelag, E. (1997). Temporal integration of the brain as studied with the metronome paradigm. In: H. Atmanspacher, E. Ruhnau (ed.). Time, temporality, now. Part.II. Experiencing time and concepts of time in an interdisciplinary perspective. Copyright Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. p.121-131.

Szelag, E., von Steinbuchel, N., Reiser, M., Langen, E. G., Poppel, E. (1996). Temporal constraints in processing of nonverbal rhythmic patterns. Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis, 56 (1), p.215-225.

Szelag, E., Kanabus, M., Kolodziejczyk, I., Kowalska, J., Szuchnik, J. (2004). Individual differences in temporal information processing in humans. Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis, 64 (3), p.349-366.

Tyron, W. W. (1982). A simplified time-series analysis for evaluation treatment interventions. Journal of applied behavioral analysis, Fall, 15 (3), p.423-429.

Takeuchi, A. H., Hulse, S. H. (1991). Absolute-pitch judgments of black- and white-key pitches. Music perception, 9, p.27-46.

Takeuchi, A. H., Hulse, S. H. (1993). Absolute pitch. Psychological bulletin, March, 113 (2), p.345-361.

Talavage, T. M., Sereno, M. I., Melcher, J. R., Ledden, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Dale, A. M. (2004). Tonotopic organization in human auditory cortex revealed by progressions of frequency sensitivity. Journal of neurophysiology, March, 91 (3), p.1282-1296.

Teki, S., Kumar, S., von Kriegstein, K., Stewart, L., Lyness, C., Moore, B. C., Capleton, B., Griffiths, T. D. (2012). Navigating the auditory scene: an expert role for the hippocampus. Journal of neuroscience, August, 32 (35), p.12251-12257.

Terhardt, E., Ward, W. D. (1982). Recognition of musical key: exploratory study. Journal of the acoustical society of America, July, 72 (1), p.26-33.

Terhardt, E., Seewann, M. (1983). Aural key identification and its relationship to absolute pitch. Music perception, 1, p.63-84.

Tervaniemi, M., Hugdahl, K. (2003). Lateralization of auditory-cortex functions. Brain research reviews, 43 (3), January, p.231-246.

Trainor, L. J., Shahin, A., Roberts, L. E. (2003). Effects of musical training on the auditory cortex in children. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, November, 999, p.506-513.

Trainor, L. J., Trehub, S. E. (1992). A comparison of infants’ and adults’ sensitivity to western musical structure. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, May, 18 (2), p.394-402.

Tramo, M. J., Cariani, P. A., Delgutte, B., Braida, L. D. (2001). Neurobiological foundations for the theory of harmony in western tonal music. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, June, 930, p.92-116.

Tramo, J. M., Cariani, P. A., Delgutte, B., Braida, L. D. (2003). Neurobiology of harmony perception. In: I. Peretz, R. J. Zatorre. The cognitive neuroscience of music. ed. New York, Oxford university press. p.126-151.

Tramo, M. J., Shah, G. D., Braida, L. D. (2002). Functional role of auditory cortex in frequency processing and pitch perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, January, 87 (1), p.122-139.

Trehub, S. (2003). Absolute and relative pitch processing in tone learning tasks. Developmental science, 6 (1), p.44-45.

Trehub, S. E., Schellenberg, E. G., Hill, D. S. (1997). The origins of music perception and cognition: a developmental perspective. In: I. Deliege, J. Sloboda (eds.) Perception and cognition of music. ed. Hove, England, Psychology Press, Erlbaum UK Taylor and Francis. p.103-128.

Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, Sh. L. M., Nusbaum, H. C. (2018). Absolute pitch can be learned by some adults. 22 p. https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Shannon_Heald/publication/325208629_Absolute_pitch_can_be_learned_ by_some_adults/links/5bc12772299bf1004c5e4bd4/Absolute-pitch-can-be-learned-by-some-adults.pdf (access on March 31st 2019).

Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, Sh. L. M., Nusbaum, H. C. (2018). Long-term pitch memory for music recordings is related to auditory working memory precision. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, April, 71 (4), p.879-891.

Van Hedger, S. C., Heald, Sh. L. M., Koch, R., Nusbaum, H. C. (2015). Auditory working memory predicts individual differences in absolute pitch learning. Cognition, July, 140, p.95-110.

Van Strien, N. M., Cappaert, N. L., Witter, M. P. (2009). The anatomy of memory: an interactive overview of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, April, 10 (4), p.272-282.

Vanzella, P., Schellenberg, E. G. (2010). Absolute pitch: effects of

timbre on note-naming ability. PloSOne, November, 5 (11), e15449. 7 p.

Vitouch, O. (2003). Absolutist models of absolute pitch are absolutely misleading. Music perception, Fall, 21 (1), p.111-117.

Wagner, A. D., Schacter, D. L., Rotte, M., Koutstaal, W., Maril, A., Dale, A. M., Rosen, B. R., Buckner, R. L. (1998). Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science, August, 281 (5380), p.1188-1191.

Ward, W. D. (1963). Absolute pitch: Part I. Sound: its uses and control, 2(3), p.14-21; 2 (4), p.33-41.

Ward, W. D. (1999). Absolute Pitch. In: D. Deutsch (ed.). The psychology of music. ed. San Diego, CA, Academic Press. p.265-298.

Ward, L. M. (2003) . Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends in cognitive sciences, December, 7 (12), p.553-559.

Ward, W. D., Burns, E. M. (1982). Absolute Pitch. In: D. Deutsch (ed.). The psychology of music. ed. San Diego, CA, Academic Press. p.431-451.

Warren, J. D., Uppenkamp, S., Patterson, R. D., Griffiths, T. D. (2003). Separating pitch chroma and pitch height in the human brain. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, August, 100 (17), p.10038-10042.

Watt, H. J. (1917). The psychology of sound. ed. London and New York, Cambridge University Press. 241 p.

Wilson, S. J., Lusher, D., Martin, C. L., Rayner, G., McLachlan, N. (2012). Intersecting factors lead to absolute pitch acquisition that is maintained in a ‘fixed do’ environment. Music Perception, 29 (3), p.285-296.

Wing, R. A. (1941) A factorial study of musical tests. British journal of psychology, April, 31 (4), p.341-355.

Wong, P. C., Uppunda, A. K., Parrish, T. B., Dhar, S. (2008). Cortical mechanisms of speech perception in noise. Journal of speech language and hearing research, August, 51 (4), p.1026-1041.

Wu, C., Kirk, I. J., Hamm, J. P., Lim, V. K. (2008). The neural networks involved in pitch labeling of absolute pitch musicians. Neuroreport, May, 19 (8), p.851-854.

Young, L. C. (1941). On randomness in ordered sequences. Annals of mathematical statistics, 12, p.293-300.

Zaehle, T., Jancke, L., Meyer, M. (2007). Electrical brain imaging evidences left auditory cortex involvement in speech and non-speech discrimination based on temporal features. Behavioral and brain functions, December, 3 (63), 11 p.

Zakay, D., Roziner, I., Ben-Arzi, S. (1984). On the nature of absolute pitch. Archiv fur psychologie, 136 (2), p.163-166.

Zatorre, R. J. (1985). Discrimination and recognition of tonal melodies after unilateral cerebral excisions. Neuropsychologia, 23 (1), p.31-41.

Zatorre, R. J. (2003). Absolute pitch: a model for understanding the influence of genes and development on neural and cognitive function. Nature neuroscience, July, 6 (7), p.692-695.

Zatorre, R. J. (2005). Neuroscience: finding the missing fundamental.

Nature, August, 436 (7054), p.1093-1094.

Zatorre, R. J., Beckett, C. (1989). Multiple coding strategies in the retention of musical tones by possessors of absolute pitch. Memory and cognition, September, 17 (5), p.582-589.

Zatorre, R. J., Samson, S. (1991). Role of the right temporal neocortex in retention of pitch in auditory short-term memory. Brain, December, 144 (6), p.2403-2417.

Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E., Gjedde, A. (1992). Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science, May, 256 (5058), p.846-849.

Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E. (1994). Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch. Journal of neuroscience, April, 14 (4), p.1908-1919.

Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer, E., Evans, A. C. (1996). Hearing in the mind's ear: a PET investigation of musical imagery and perception. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, Winter, 8 (1), p. 29-46.

Zatorre, R. J., Perry, D. W., Beckett, C. A., Westbury, C. F., Evans, A. C. (1998). Functional anatomy of musical processing in listeners with absolute pitch and relative pitch. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the United States of America, 95 (6), p.3172-3177.

Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cerebral cortex, October, 11 (10), p.946-953.

Downloads

Published

2019-10-21

How to Cite

Absolute Pitch Acquisition as Memory System with Its’ Musical Executive Function from Cognitive Neuropsychological Perspective. (2019). Asian Journal of Humanities and Social Studies, 7(5). https://doi.org/10.24203/ajhss.v7i5.5900

Similar Articles

21-30 of 105

You may also start an advanced similarity search for this article.